Vis enkel innførsel

dc.contributor.authorBreivik, Gunnar
dc.contributor.authorSand, Trond Svela
dc.contributor.authorSookermany, Anders McDonald
dc.date.accessioned2020-02-05T11:41:32Z
dc.date.available2020-02-05T11:41:32Z
dc.date.created2017-09-19T00:45:58Z
dc.date.issued2017
dc.identifier.citationPersonality and Individual Differences. 2017, 119, 266-272.nb_NO
dc.identifier.issn0191-8869
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/11250/2639788
dc.descriptionI Brage finner du siste tekst-versjon av artikkelen, og den kan inneholde ubetydelige forskjeller fra forlagets pdf-versjon. Forlagets pdf-versjon finner du på sciencedirect.com / In Brage you'll find the final text version of the article, and it may contain insignificant differences from the journal's pdf version. The definitive version is available at sciencedirect.comnb_NO
dc.description.abstractThe article is based on a comprehensive study of the relation between sensation seeking and various forms of risk-taking in a representative sample of the adult Norwegian population, aged 15 years and above (N = 1000). The study included social, intellectual, financial, achievement-related, political, physical, ethical and existential risktaking dimensions. There was an expected main effect for age as well as gender on total sensation seeking, but no main effect for social class. All sensation seeking scales correlated positively with all the risk dimensions, although moderately for most scales. Physical risk had the highest correlation scores. The study found that altogether 21, 2% of the general population had been involved in risky activities during their life and had elevated scores on sensation seeking. A relatively high percentage of the population would be willing to be involved in risky sports (35,7%), risky jobs (54,8%) or risky military operations (25,9%), provided they were in good shape and of the right age. Those who were willing had higher sensation seeking scores. Only 16,9% of the population thought basejumping or other risk sports should be prohibited. The prohibitionists had lower sensation seeking scores than the non-prohibitionists.nb_NO
dc.language.isoengnb_NO
dc.relation.urihttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886917304889
dc.subjectsensation seekingnb_NO
dc.subjectrisk-takingnb_NO
dc.subjectrisk sportsnb_NO
dc.subjectrisky jobsnb_NO
dc.subjectmilitary operationsnb_NO
dc.titleSensation seeking and risk-taking in the Norwegian populationnb_NO
dc.title.alternativeSensation seeking and risk-taking in the Norwegian populationnb_NO
dc.typeJournal articlenb_NO
dc.typePeer reviewednb_NO
dc.description.versionacceptedVersionnb_NO
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Idrettspedagogikk og -psykologi: 333nb_NO
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Physical education and sport psychology: 333nb_NO
dc.source.pagenumber266-272nb_NO
dc.source.volume119nb_NO
dc.source.journalPersonality and Individual Differencesnb_NO
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.039
dc.identifier.cristin1495108
dc.description.localcodeSeksjon for kultur og samfunn / Department of Cultural and Social Studiesnb_NO
cristin.unitcode150,33,0,0
cristin.unitnameSeksjon for kultur og samfunn
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextpostprint
cristin.qualitycode1


Tilhørende fil(er)

Thumbnail

Denne innførselen finnes i følgende samling(er)

Vis enkel innførsel